Elsevier

Computers in Human Behavior

Volume 63, October 2016, Pages 509-516
Computers in Human Behavior

Full length article
Fear of missing out, need for touch, anxiety and depression are related to problematic smartphone use

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.05.079Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Problematic smartphone use was most related to the fear of missing out, depression (inversely), and a need for touch.

  • Behavior activation mediated associations between smartphone use and both anxiety and depression.

  • Results demonstrate the importance of social and tactile need fulfillment variables that explain problem smartphone use.

Abstract

Problematic smartphone use is an important public health challenge and is linked with poor mental health outcomes. However, little is known about the mechanisms that maintain this behavior. We recruited a sample of 308 participants from Amazon’s Mechanical Turk labor market. Participants responded to standardized measures of problematic smartphone use, and frequency of smartphone use, depression and anxiety and possible mechanisms including behavioral activation, need for touch, fear of missing out (FoMO), and emotion regulation. Problematic smartphone use was most correlated with anxiety, need for touch and FoMO. The frequency of use was most correlated (inversely) with depression. In regression models, problematic smartphone use was associated with FoMO, depression (inversely), anxiety, and need for touch. Frequency of use was associated with need for touch, and (inversely) with depressive symptoms. Behavioral activation mediated associations between smartphone use (both problematic and usage frequency) and depression and anxiety symptoms. Emotional suppression also mediated the association between problematic smartphone use and anxiety. Results demonstrate the importance of social and tactile need fulfillment variables such as FoMO and need for touch as critical mechanisms that can explain problematic smartphone use and its association with depression and anxiety.

Introduction

Smartphones are ubiquitous in modern day society globally. Pew Research polling indicates that 72% of Americans own a smartphone, with a global average ownership of 43% (Poushter, 2016, February 22). However, for many people, problematic smartphone use has harmful effects. For example, research indicates hazardous outcomes for problematic smartphone use, including distracting drivers and pedestrians (reviewed in Cazzulino et al., 2014, Thompson et al., 2013). Additionally effects include musculoskeletal health effects (Xie et al., 2016, İNal et al., 2015), poor physical fitness (Lepp et al., 2013, Rebold et al., 2016) and academic deficits (Lepp et al., 2014, Prabu et al., 2015). Studying problematic smartphone use is therefore of significant public health significance. Our focus in the present paper is on correlates and mechanisms of problematic smartphone use involving psychopathology, dysfunctional self- and emotional control, and social and tactile need fulfillment.

Evidence for the construct of problematic smartphone use, or smartphone addiction, comes from a growing literature base. In Pew Research polling, nearly half of Americans reported that they “couldn’t live without” their smartphones (Smith & Page, 2015, April 1). When separated from one’s smartphone in experimental studies, many participants evidence mounting anxiety (Cheever, Rosen, Carrier, & Chavez, 2014) and physiological increases in heart rate and blood pressure (Clayton, Leshner, & Almond, 2015). Furthermore, phantom cell phone vibrations are commonly reported, despite an absence of incoming phone notifications (Kruger & Djerf, 2016). These dependency-like behaviors and withdrawal-like symptoms are not surprising given how much people rely on their phones in daily life for productivity, information seeking, and social interaction, among other things (van Deursen, Bolle, Hegner, & Kommers, 2015).

Recent studies identified the psychopathological correlates of problematic smartphone use. Methodologically, this literature base has generous sample sizes (n > 200), primarily using student participants collected in the U.S., China and Korea, with standardized measures of problematic mobile phone use. The majority of these studies are cross-sectional, using regression or structural equation modeling analyses. Most widely reported are relationships between problematic smartphone use and severity of depression and anxiety symptoms. Other research has shown associations between problematic smartphone use and more general stress and self-esteem, but with less consistent findings (e.g., Smetaniuk, 2014, van Deursen et al., 2015). The association between smartphone use and depression typically ranges from 0.3 to 0.5 for bivariate correlations and standardized regression coefficients when adjusting for other relevant variables such as age and gender (most recently in Demirci et al., 2015, Harwood et al., 2014; J. Kim et al., 2015a, Smetaniuk, 2014). Anxiety symptoms are associated with problematic smartphone use on a bivariate and multivariate basis with coefficients averaging 0.2 (recently in Demirci et al., 2015, Harwood et al., 2014; R. Kim et al., 2015b, Lee et al., 2014).

Thus the literature demonstrates solid evidence for small to medium effect size associations between problematic smartphone use and depression and anxiety. There is evidence from prospective studies for bidirectional relations, whereby excessive smartphone use can result in psychopathology, which in turn drives smartphone use (van den Eijnden et al., 2008, Thomee et al., 2011, Yen et al., 2012).

Theoretical explanations specific to problematic smartphone use are limited. Very recently, Billieux and colleagues developed a theoretical model to explain problematic mobile phone use (Billieux, Maurage, Lopez-Fernandez, Kuss, & Griffiths, 2015). Their model is based on primary pathways to problematic use including excessive reassurance seeking, impulsivity, and extraversion. We examine variables falling within this model in the prediction of problematic smartphone use, but we also extend this model by focusing on additional, newer variables with theoretical support that have only recently been tested empirically.

First, within Billieux et al.’s (2015) extraversion pathway, fear of missing out (FoMO) is a newer personality construct involving reluctance to miss important information, including social information. FoMO results in the need to frequently stay connected to social networks. FoMO was first discussed in the news media (Fake, 2011, March 15; Morford, 2010, August 4). People high in FoMO likely overuse their smartphones to satisfy the need to stay connected. FoMO appears to drive overuse of social media based on web surveys with college students and community participants (Alt, 2015, Przybylski et al., 2013). Among college students, FoMO was associated with increases in problematic smartphone use in a laboratory study (Clayton et al., 2015).

Another relevant construct, within the impulsive pathway to problematic smartphone use, is decreased emotional self-control, or emotional dysregulation. Dysregulated emotion is often defined by two processes – decreased cognitive reappraisal, and increased emotional suppression (Gross and John, 2003, Gross, 1998). Problematic smartphone users likely overuse their phones in part because of an inability to regulate their emotions. Additionally, problematic smartphone use may be a technique (albeit ineffective) to deal with or regulate negative emotion. In fact, Hoffner and Lee (2015) found in a survey study with undergraduates that habitual use of emotional suppression was associated with more intense missing of particular aspects of smartphone use, including entertainment and information content. A host of research has shown that the relationship between problematic behaviors and associated mental disorders is mediated by emotional dysregulation (reviewed in Weiss, Sullivan, & Tull, 2015). We were particularly interested in whether emotional dysregulation mediates relations between problematic smartphone use in accounting for depression and anxiety.

We propose an additional variable to Billieux et al.’s (2015) model. One addictive aspect of smartphone use is the pleasure derived from tactile sensations in holding the phone, and the autotelic touch (Peck & Childers, 2003a) required in completing tasks with one’s fingers (Lee et al., 2014). “Need for touch” (Peck & Childers, 2003b) is a construct from the marketing field describing a personality variable of desiring haptic information through the hands. People high in this trait are more likely to analytically and experientially sample a product’s features (Yazdanparast & Spears, 2012). Research demonstrates that if experiential product sampling is satisfying and fun, people high in the need for touch are likely to engage in impulse purchasing (Peck and Childers, 2006, Vieria, 2012). People high in the need for touch may demonstrate an overuse of a smartphone’s touch screen to satisfy this need. Lee et al. (2014) found in a Taiwanese community survey that problematic smartphone use was significantly related to need for touch.

We also assess potential mechanisms that can account for relations between problem smartphone use and both depression and anxiety. One candidate mechanism is behavioral activation. According to the Behavioral Model of Depression, low levels of positive reinforcement are responsible for depressive symptoms, and increasing positive reinforcement can be obtained by increasing the number and types of gratifying/pleasurable events in one’s environment (Lewinsohn, 1974). An elaboration of this model, the Integrated Model of Depression, adds a more comprehensive interplay of environmental and dispositional factors to developing depression, such as environmental stressors and maladaptive cognitions (Lewinsohn, Hoberman, Teri, & Hautzinger, 1985). Behavioral activation involves engagement in adaptive gratifying/pleasurable activities as a functional response alternative to avoidance. Behavioral activation has shown great promise as a target for alleviation of clinical depression (reviewed in Dimidjian, Barrera, Martell, Munoz, & Lewinsohn, 2011). It has also been found effective for other disorders with depression content, such as posttraumatic stress disorder (Acierno et al., 2016), as well as using different delivery formats, such as via the internet (Carlbring et al., 2013). Yet unexplored is whether increased behavioral activation offsets the impact of problematic smartphone use on depression and anxiety outcomes.

In the present study, we had several hypotheses about variables predicting problematic smartphone use and usage frequency outcomes:

Hypothesis 1) We hypothesized that depression severity would be significantly, positively related to the smartphone use outcome variables, with moderate effect sizes. This hypothesis is based on prior research finding such effects for depression severity in relation these smartphone use variables (Demirci et al., 2015, Harwood et al., 2014; J. Kim et al., 2015a, Smetaniuk, 2014).

Hypothesis 2) We expected that anxiety severity would be significantly, positively related to our smartphone use outcomes, with small to medium effects. This hypothesis is based on findings from prior studies examining these relationships (Demirci et al., 2015, Harwood et al., 2014; R. Kim et al., 2015b, Lee et al., 2014).

Hypothesis 3) We anticipated that behavioral activation would be inversely related to the smartphone use outcome variables. Decreased behavioral activation is related to greater depression severity (Dimidjian et al., 2011), and depression is related to problematic smartphone use (Demirci et al., 2015, Harwood et al., 2014; J. Kim et al., 2015a, Smetaniuk, 2014). Thus as with depression, we expected moderate (but inverse) effects for behavioral activation, whereby people with less behavioral activation should use their smartphones more.

Therefore, we were interested in testing these psychopathology variables in relation to our smartphone use variables. Additionally, we were interested in testing the newer variables that have theoretical but only limited empirical inquiry for relations with smartphone use: emotion regulation, FoMO and need for touch.

Hypothesis 4) We hypothesized that emotion regulation would be significantly, inversely related to our smartphone use outcomes. Maladaptive emotion regulation processes (or dysregulation) fall within Billieux et al.’s (2015) impulsive pathway to problematic smartphone use. Specific types of emotion dysregulation - decreased cognitive reappraisal and increased emotional suppression (Gross and John, 2003, Gross, 1998) - are increasingly important variables in explaining clinical psychopathology (Weiss et al., 2015). And thus the use of decreased cognitive reappraisal (Hypothesis 4a) and increased emotional suppression (4b) should be related to increases in our smartphone use outcomes based on recent research (Hoffner & Lee, 2015).

Hypothesis 5) We expected that FoMO would be significantly, positively related to the smartphone use outcomes. FoMO falls within Billieaux et al.’s (2015) extraversion pathway toward problematic smartphone use. FoMO should be at least moderately related to the smartphone use outcome variables (Clayton et al., 2015) because it involves social need fulfillment which an important predictor of problematic smartphone use (Lopez-Fernandez, Honrubia-Serrano, Freixa-Blanxart, & Gibson, 2014).

Hypothesis 6) We hypothesized that need for touch would be significantly, positively related to the smartphone use outcome variables. Need for touch involves the fulfillment of tactile needs. And tactile need fulfillment is related to increased smartphone use (Lee et al., 2014).

We were also interested in examining the incremental contribution of these newer variables that have only recently been examined for associations with smartphone use.

Hypothesis 7) We expected that a block of variables including emotion dysregulation, FoMO and need for touch would contribute unique variance, above psychopathology, in association with the smartphone use variables. These variables as a whole involve deficits in emotional coping, and social and tactile need fulfillment. Because smartphone use is important in need fulfillment for emotional (Clayton et al., 2015, Hoffner and Lee, 2015), social (Clayton et al., 2015, Lopez-Fernandez et al., 2014) and tactile needs (Lee et al., 2014), we expected to find support for this hypothesis.

Finally, we were interested in testing mediation effects for behavioral activation and emotion regulation scores.

Hypothesis 8) We hypothesized that behavioral activation would mediate relations between smartphone usage and both depression and anxiety severity. That is, the relationship between greater smartphone use and greater depression/anxiety would be accounted for through decreased behavioral activation. This mediation hypothesis is exploratory, without prior research investigating this exact question.

Hypothesis 9) We expected that maladaptive emotion regulation would mediate relations between our smartphone use variables and both depression and anxiety. That is, relations between increased smartphone usage and increased depression/anxiety would be accounted for by the less adaptive use of emotion regulation skills (i.e., lower cognitive reappraisal and higher emotional suppression). This mediation hypothesis is exploratory, having not been investigated before.

Section snippets

Procedure

In early 2016, we recruited participants from Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (Mturk), an online labor market often used in social science research (Shapiro, Chandler, & Mueller, 2013). Though a convenience sample, Mturk offers several advantages in data collection, discussed by Landers and Behrend (2015). We chose Mturk to obtain avid technology users most likely to own a smartphone. We described the study on Mturk as a 15–20-min investigation of mobile phone and web service use, offering 75 cents

Results

Descriptive statistics are presented for the primary measures in Table 1, including means and standard deviations for the primary variables. Measuring smartphone use frequency, the most prevalent features were text/instant messaging (M = 5.15, SD = 1.20), internet/websites (M = 5.03, SD = 1.17), email (M = 4.64, SD = 1.36), and social network sites (M = 4.41, SD = 1.62). Table 1 also presents zero-order Pearson correlations between the study variables. Smartphone use frequency and SAS scores

Discussion

The present study examined variables conceptually related to problematic smartphone use and use frequency. The results were largely consistent with hypotheses, offering an additional dimension for consideration in problematic smartphone use (need for touch), indicators which can separate use frequency from problematic use, as well as a potential target for intervention via a mediated relationship through emotion regulation and behavioral activation. The results are discussed in greater detail

Acknowledgments

No external funding supported this study.

References (56)

  • X. Lu et al.

    Internet and mobile phone text-messaging dependency: factor structure and correlation with dysphoric mood among Japanese adults

    Computers in Human Behavior

    (2011)
  • R.C. Manos et al.

    The behavioral activation scale for depression-short form: development and validation

    Behavior Therapy

    (2011)
  • J. Peck et al.

    If I touch it I have to have it: individual and environmental influences on impulse purchasing

    Journal of Business Research

    (2006)
  • A.K. Przybylski et al.

    Motivational, emotional, and behavioral correlates of fear of missing out

    Computers in Human Behavior

    (2013)
  • M.J. Rebold et al.

    The impact of cell phone texting on the amount of time spent exercising at different intensities

    Computers in Human Behavior

    (2016)
  • N.H. Weiss et al.

    Explicating the role of emotion dysregulation in risky behaviors: a review and synthesis of the literature with directions for future research and clinical practice

    Current Opinion in Psychology

    (2015)
  • J.Y. Yen et al.

    The bidirectional interactions between addiction, behaviour approach and behaviour inhibition systems among adolescents in a prospective study

    Psychiatry Research

    (2012)
  • R. Acierno et al.

    Behavioral activation and therapeutic exposure for posttraumatic stress disorder: a noninferiority trial of treatment delivered in person versus home-based telehealth

    Depression and Anxiety

    (2016)
  • M.M. Antony et al.

    Psychometric properties of the 42-item and 21-item versions of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales in clinical groups and a community sample

    Psychological Assessment

    (1998)
  • J. Billieux et al.

    Can disordered mobile phone use be considered a behavioral addiction? An update on current evidence and a comprehensive model for future research

    Current Addiction Reports

    (2015)
  • F. Cazzulino et al.

    Cell phones and young drivers: a systematic review regarding the association between psychological factors and prevention

    Traffic Injury Prevention

    (2014)
  • R.B. Clayton et al.

    The extended iSelf: the impact of iPhone separation on cognition, emotion, and physiology

    Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication

    (2015)
  • C.M. Cummings et al.

    Comorbidity of anxiety and depression in children and adolescents: 20 years after

    Psychological Bulletin

    (2014)
  • K. Demirci et al.

    Relationship of smartphone use severity with sleep quality, depression, and anxiety in university students

    Journal of Behavioral Addictions

    (2015)
  • S. Dimidjian et al.

    The origins and current status of behavioral activation treatments for depression

    Annual Review of Clinical Psychology

    (2011)
  • C. Fake

    FOMO and social media

    (2011, March 15)
  • J.J. Gross

    The emerging field of emotion regulation: an integrative review

    Review of General Psychology

    (1998)
  • J.J. Gross et al.

    Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (2003)
  • Cited by (533)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text